Cognitive cross-over: Implications for a theory of social organizing

Research output: Chapter in Book/Report/Conference proceedingBook chapterResearchpeer-review

Standard

Cognitive cross-over : Implications for a theory of social organizing. / Cowley, Stephen J.; Kirkeby, Johanne.

Organizational Cognition: The Theory of Social Organizing. ed. / Davide Secchi; Rasmus Gahrn-Andersen; Stephen J. Cowley. New York : Routledge, 2022. p. 239-258.

Research output: Chapter in Book/Report/Conference proceedingBook chapterResearchpeer-review

Harvard

Cowley, SJ & Kirkeby, J 2022, Cognitive cross-over: Implications for a theory of social organizing. in D Secchi, R Gahrn-Andersen & SJ Cowley (eds), Organizational Cognition: The Theory of Social Organizing. Routledge, New York, pp. 239-258.

APA

Cowley, S. J., & Kirkeby, J. (2022). Cognitive cross-over: Implications for a theory of social organizing. In D. Secchi, R. Gahrn-Andersen, & S. J. Cowley (Eds.), Organizational Cognition: The Theory of Social Organizing (pp. 239-258). Routledge.

Vancouver

Cowley SJ, Kirkeby J. Cognitive cross-over: Implications for a theory of social organizing. In Secchi D, Gahrn-Andersen R, Cowley SJ, editors, Organizational Cognition: The Theory of Social Organizing. New York: Routledge. 2022. p. 239-258

Author

Cowley, Stephen J. ; Kirkeby, Johanne. / Cognitive cross-over : Implications for a theory of social organizing. Organizational Cognition: The Theory of Social Organizing. editor / Davide Secchi ; Rasmus Gahrn-Andersen ; Stephen J. Cowley. New York : Routledge, 2022. pp. 239-258

Bibtex

@inbook{07313a3cf2f54921923bb397d985b806,
title = "Cognitive cross-over: Implications for a theory of social organizing",
abstract = "Cognitive science often uses a logic focused on what we do as opposed to one that asks how things come about. In reversing what Ingold calls the logic of inversion, the chapter makes three contributions based on a systemic view. First, it presents case studies to clarify how people improvise epistemic outcomes. Using systemic ethnography, we initially extend Hutchins{\textquoteright} account of how a crew programs a flight path. Then, we turn to how epistemic outcomes arise in two minutes of goalkeeper training. We thus clarify both how change can be managed and how it can draw on joint and spontaneous control. Second, the social practices exemplify cognitive cross-over or how “reciprocal sensitivity” enables emplaced parties to bring forth “epistemic outcomes.” Information transfer and neural resonance co-function with emplaced flux as people use experience to shape {\textquoteleft}what happens.{\textquoteright} In the examples, the results unite what people do (in a social unit) with a concern for doing things right. Together, bodies generate intent as they reach outcomes. Third, we turn to social organizing: We trace cognitive cross-over to ecologies where communication and coordination presuppose public microcognition. Spontaneity serves not only to display, explore, and get things right but, just as importantly, to build soft skills as parties develop flexibility, resilience, and adaptivity. Hence human practices depend on social organizing as persons prompt selves and each other to achieve epistemic outcomes.",
author = "Cowley, {Stephen J.} and Johanne Kirkeby",
year = "2022",
month = sep,
day = "30",
language = "English",
isbn = "9780367769512",
pages = "239--258",
editor = "Secchi, {Davide } and Gahrn-Andersen, {Rasmus } and Cowley, {Stephen J. }",
booktitle = "Organizational Cognition",
publisher = "Routledge",
address = "United Kingdom",

}

RIS

TY - CHAP

T1 - Cognitive cross-over

T2 - Implications for a theory of social organizing

AU - Cowley, Stephen J.

AU - Kirkeby, Johanne

PY - 2022/9/30

Y1 - 2022/9/30

N2 - Cognitive science often uses a logic focused on what we do as opposed to one that asks how things come about. In reversing what Ingold calls the logic of inversion, the chapter makes three contributions based on a systemic view. First, it presents case studies to clarify how people improvise epistemic outcomes. Using systemic ethnography, we initially extend Hutchins’ account of how a crew programs a flight path. Then, we turn to how epistemic outcomes arise in two minutes of goalkeeper training. We thus clarify both how change can be managed and how it can draw on joint and spontaneous control. Second, the social practices exemplify cognitive cross-over or how “reciprocal sensitivity” enables emplaced parties to bring forth “epistemic outcomes.” Information transfer and neural resonance co-function with emplaced flux as people use experience to shape ‘what happens.’ In the examples, the results unite what people do (in a social unit) with a concern for doing things right. Together, bodies generate intent as they reach outcomes. Third, we turn to social organizing: We trace cognitive cross-over to ecologies where communication and coordination presuppose public microcognition. Spontaneity serves not only to display, explore, and get things right but, just as importantly, to build soft skills as parties develop flexibility, resilience, and adaptivity. Hence human practices depend on social organizing as persons prompt selves and each other to achieve epistemic outcomes.

AB - Cognitive science often uses a logic focused on what we do as opposed to one that asks how things come about. In reversing what Ingold calls the logic of inversion, the chapter makes three contributions based on a systemic view. First, it presents case studies to clarify how people improvise epistemic outcomes. Using systemic ethnography, we initially extend Hutchins’ account of how a crew programs a flight path. Then, we turn to how epistemic outcomes arise in two minutes of goalkeeper training. We thus clarify both how change can be managed and how it can draw on joint and spontaneous control. Second, the social practices exemplify cognitive cross-over or how “reciprocal sensitivity” enables emplaced parties to bring forth “epistemic outcomes.” Information transfer and neural resonance co-function with emplaced flux as people use experience to shape ‘what happens.’ In the examples, the results unite what people do (in a social unit) with a concern for doing things right. Together, bodies generate intent as they reach outcomes. Third, we turn to social organizing: We trace cognitive cross-over to ecologies where communication and coordination presuppose public microcognition. Spontaneity serves not only to display, explore, and get things right but, just as importantly, to build soft skills as parties develop flexibility, resilience, and adaptivity. Hence human practices depend on social organizing as persons prompt selves and each other to achieve epistemic outcomes.

M3 - Book chapter

SN - 9780367769512

SP - 239

EP - 258

BT - Organizational Cognition

A2 - Secchi, Davide

A2 - Gahrn-Andersen, Rasmus

A2 - Cowley, Stephen J.

PB - Routledge

CY - New York

ER -

ID: 321280696